1. Introduction
Physical manifestations of anthropogenic climate change are abundantly clear to scientists across many disciplines, informing their near-unanimous agreement on its reality (Cook et al. 2016; USGCRP 2017). Agreement among the U.S. public, on the other hand, is generally lower and tied to political identity. Public acceptance of anthropogenic climate change has, nevertheless, risen gradually from percentages in the low 50s in 2010 to the mid-60s by 2016 (Hamilton 2016). Scientific reports and communication undoubtedly contribute to this rise. Experiencing physical manifestations of climate change might contribute to public acceptance as well. Evidence for lasting impacts of physical events on public beliefs about climate change has been mixed, however, and complicated by political effects.
Climate-change opinions show at least transient impacts from environmental factors such as weather trends (Hamilton and Keim 2009; Kaufmann et al. 2017; Shao et al. 2014, 2016), daily weather or ambient conditions (Broomell et al. 2017; Hamilton and Lemcke-Stampone 2014; Joireman et al. 2010; Ripberger et al. 2017; Risen and Critcher 2011; Zaval et al. 2014), and extreme events (Cutler 2015, 2016; Shao 2016) around the respondent’s location. Weather may particularly impress individuals who hold less committed political or climate-change beliefs (Egan and Mullin 2012; Hamilton and Stampone 2013; Myers et al. 2013). Researchers have explored a variety of different weather, climate, or environmental indicators as possible predictors of views on local or global change. With some indicators and datasets, they detect no significant climate or environmental effects, or find them to be weak compared with political effects (Brulle et al. 2012; Carlton et al. 2016; Carmichael et al. 2017; McCright et al. 2014; Marlon et al. 2018; Marquart-Pyatt et al. 2014).
Much of this work employs objective indicators of weather or climate, derived from sources such as weather records, news accounts, or disaster expenditures. Subjective recollections regarding weather or climate correlate with climate-change beliefs as well (Akerlof et al. 2013; Borick and Rabe 2010; Capstick and Pidgeon 2014; Krosnick et al. 2006; Lee et al. 2015; Li et al. 2011; Weber 2010). This is consistent with weather perceptions affecting beliefs but also with the reverse: through motivated reasoning or following cues from media and political leaders, general climate-change beliefs can shape people’s perceptions of actual weather (Borick and Rabe 2017; Howe and Leiserowitz 2013; Myers et al. 2013; Shao 2016). The clearest evidence for beliefs constraining weather/climate perceptions comes from studies where political identity or climate beliefs predict individuals’ accuracy in characterizing known local trends in temperature or disasters (Hamilton et al. 2016a,b), and even their recollections of record-breaking weather that occurred a few weeks earlier (Hamilton and Lemcke-Stampone 2016). This pattern is ideologically asymmetric: conservatives and individuals who reject the reality of anthropogenic climate change are more inclined to report past weather that seems consistent with their position, whether accurately or not (Borick and Rabe 2017; Hamilton and Lemcke-Stampone 2016; Hamilton et al. 2016a,b; Howe and Leiserowitz 2013).
Gradual shifts in average temperature marking climate change, a few degrees over decades, are much smaller in magnitude than daily or seasonal variations. Many people might not personally notice the warming, if temperatures were the only change. Warming can bring along more obvious local changes, however, such as shortened seasons in a ski town, or earlier ice-out on a frozen lake. The timing of impactful transitions between water and snow or ice, in particular, provides mundane signals of change that should be noticeable regardless of political orientation.
This paper tests political identity along with other individual characteristics as predictors of weather/climate perceptions in a historically snowy rural region. In summer 2017, Carsey School researchers conducted a random-sample telephone survey of 1650 residents in the rural North Country of northern New England. Mainly covering nonclimate topics, the survey also carried a question asking respondents whether they thought that recent winters there had been warm compared with earlier decades. Objectively, winter warming over this period had been substantial, with broad consequences (Wake et al. 2014). Analysis of the North Country survey adds a new case study that extends previous work on how political identity affects individuals’ accuracy in characterizing a known local trend.
2. The North Country of northern New England
New England’s rural North Country (northern New Hampshire, northeast Vermont, and northwest Maine) differs from the more populous and economically diverse parts of New England. Geographic remoteness, rugged topography, and historically harsh winters have limited the appeal of this region as a place to live for many, but the vast forests and powerful rivers offered low-skill manufacturing job opportunities in forest products industries, particularly pulp and paper. Hit hard over the past several decades by the national decline in natural-resource and manufacturing jobs, North Country communities of northern New Hampshire and bordering areas of Maine and Vermont faced challenges in restructuring their economies (Baily and Bosworth 2014; Glasmeier and Salant 2006).
Figure 1 maps the four adjacent North Country counties surveyed in summer 2017: Coös and Grafton in New Hampshire, Oxford in Maine, and Essex in Vermont. A 2008 study classified Coös and Oxford counties as “amenity/decline” areas, a common pattern in rural America where historically resource-dependent places experience decline in their traditional industries, even while natural amenities present new opportunities for growth related to tourism or amenity-based in-migration (Hamilton et al. 2008). Complicating the transition for many places, there often is outmigration by young adults seeking jobs and financial stability elsewhere, as new businesses in rural areas tend toward seasonal employment or require different kinds of skills. Essex County, not part of the 2008 study, fits this amenity/decline description as well. Grafton County shares their mountainous landscapes and northern climate, but its more diverse economy includes substantial tourism, education, high-tech, and health-sector employment, placing it farther along the transition.

Four counties covered by the 2017 North Country survey (from Hamilton et al. 2017).
Citation: Weather, Climate, and Society 10, 4; 10.1175/WCAS-D-18-0020.1

Four counties covered by the 2017 North Country survey (from Hamilton et al. 2017).
Citation: Weather, Climate, and Society 10, 4; 10.1175/WCAS-D-18-0020.1
Four counties covered by the 2017 North Country survey (from Hamilton et al. 2017).
Citation: Weather, Climate, and Society 10, 4; 10.1175/WCAS-D-18-0020.1
a. North Country winters
With the decline in traditional natural-resource dependent industries, cold-season snow and ice cover have become an economic resource. The winter recreation economy of northern New England generates more than $1 billion annually and supports thousands of local jobs (Burakowski and Magnusson 2012; Dawson and Scott 2013; Scott et al. 2008; Southwick Associates 2017). Because winter tourism for skiing, snowmobiling, and ice fishing depends on snow and ice cover, the industry and communities that rely on it are vulnerable to changes in regional cold-season climate (Dawson et al. 2013; Scott et al. 2008; Wobus et al. 2017). Economic and recreational impacts from such changes are being felt already (Bever 2017; Hamilton et al. 2003, 2007; Perry et al. 2018).
Over the past century, northern New England winters have become warmer and wetter with fewer cold extremes (Kunkel et al. 2013; Thibeault and Seth 2014; U.S. EPA 2016). The number of days with minimum temperatures below freezing has decreased by two weeks, and the coldest winter nights are warming. The number of snow-covered days has decreased, while the length of the growing season became several weeks longer. More than a century of observations shows spring ice-out dates on major lakes (Umbagog and First Connecticut) occurring 7 to 10 days earlier than in the past (Wake et al. 2014).
Figure 2a graphs December through March average temperatures calculated from NOAA (2018) data for climate divisions 1 in Vermont, New Hampshire, and Maine, with a horizontal line marking the 1901–2000 mean. The lowess smoothed curve traces gradual change, exhibiting the early twentieth-century warming, midcentury pause, and 1970s takeoff pattern characteristic of global warming (IPCC 2013). North Country winters have warmed at more than twice the global rate since 1970, making this one of the fastest warming regions in the country (Thibeault and Seth 2014; U.S. EPA 2016; Vose et al. 2014). As average temperatures rose, the extremes shifted as well, so that the warmest winters recently are warmer, and coldest winters warmer as well, compared with earlier in the twentieth century.

(a) North Country winter temperature, with reference line for twentieth-century average, and responses to North Country survey questions about (b) winter warming and (c) climate change.
Citation: Weather, Climate, and Society 10, 4; 10.1175/WCAS-D-18-0020.1

(a) North Country winter temperature, with reference line for twentieth-century average, and responses to North Country survey questions about (b) winter warming and (c) climate change.
Citation: Weather, Climate, and Society 10, 4; 10.1175/WCAS-D-18-0020.1
(a) North Country winter temperature, with reference line for twentieth-century average, and responses to North Country survey questions about (b) winter warming and (c) climate change.
Citation: Weather, Climate, and Society 10, 4; 10.1175/WCAS-D-18-0020.1
Since the mid-twentieth century, warming North Country winters’ most visible consequences include decreases in snowfall, snow depth, snow extent, and seasonal lake ice cover, as well as earlier melting (Dudley et al. 2017; Hodgkins 2013; Knowles 2015; Kunkel et al. 2009, 2013, 2016; U.S. EPA 2016; Wake et al. 2014). These observed trends are projected to continue through the end of the century as northern interior winters become milder and shorter with fewer frost days and days with snow on the ground (Maloney et al. 2014; Thibeault and Seth 2014).
b. North Country summer 2017 survey
In recent years, anecdotal reports brought up two contrasting attitudes among North Country residents. Some were optimistic about prospects for the region’s communities, whereas others saw economic or social problems getting worse, and felt that the overall situation was deteriorating. To understand the balance and social bases of these views, Carsey School researchers conducted a telephone survey that interviewed 1650 North Country residents in June and July 2017. Respondents answered questions about their perceptions, hopes, and concerns for home communities and region. Some of the same questions had been asked with earlier surveys in 2007 and 2010, providing a longitudinal perspective on what has changed or stayed much the same (Colocousis and Young 2011; Hamilton et al. 2017). Trained interviewers at the Survey Center of the University of New Hampshire called randomly selected cell and landline telephone numbers of the four target counties [response rate 19% by definition 4 in AAPOR (2016)]. Probability weights, used for all analyses here, make minor adjustments toward representativeness in terms of number of adults and telephone numbers within households, county population, and respondent age, sex, and education.
One question on the 2017 survey, placed before any mention of climate change or politics, asked whether respondents thought that winters in this region over the past 20 years had been warmer, cooler, or about average compared with 30 or 40 years ago. Sixty-one percent accurately said “warmer” (Fig. 2b). This was followed by a standard climate-change question, where sixty-five percent agreed that climate change is happening now, caused mainly by human activities (Fig. 2c). Later items asked respondents their political orientation, and whether most of their friends prefer the same political party they do. We included this friends question to test a proposition from reports that Americans increasingly choose to associate socially with people of their own political persuasion, which accentuates polarization (Iyengar and Westwood 2015; Pew Research Center 2016, 2017).
Table 1 gives the wording of these and other questions analyzed here, along with response summaries and codes used in later analysis. Table 1 also describes background characteristics (age, sex, education) that previous research found relevant to climate-change views, including a four-party political indicator that distinguishes Tea Party supporters. The Tea Party, a more conservative wing of the U.S. Republican Party, emerged in 2009 following the election of Barack Obama. Although the Tea Party organization is decentralized and somewhat informal, self-identified supporters express distinctive views on a wide range of issues, including rejection of human-caused climate change (Hamilton and Saito 2015; Leiserowitz et al. 2011; Shao 2017). Hamilton et al. (2017) provide background on the North Country survey, and draw comparisons with earlier surveys in this region. Fogg et al. (2017) chart responses to all of the 2017 survey questions, which mainly focus on nonclimate topics.
Variable definitions with codes used in regression analysis and weighted summary statistics (North Country survey June/July 2017, n = 1650). Winter asked before climate; response order for both questions rotated in interviews.


H1: Political identity influences perceptions of local weather, such that conservatives less often recognize the trend toward warmer winters.
H2: Although the winters question does not mention climate change, political identity influences both winter and climate-change responses in similar ways.
Adjusting for political effects, we test basic propositions related to length of experiential perspective.H3: Political-identity effects on winter or climate responses are stronger among respondents whose friends prefer mostly the same party.
H4: Older and long-term residents are more aware of decadal warming in this region.
3. Survey results
Sixty-one percent of our 2017 North Country respondents accurately reported that winters over the past 20 years have been warmer, on average, than winters 30 or 40 years ago. Given the many ways that winters in this region affect livelihoods, recreation, and daily life, it is not surprising that most people recognize the warming trend. But for those who did not, is this simply a matter of information, with some younger people, newcomers, or others (reasonably enough) lacking the perspective to compare seasons across decades? As a first analytical step, we break down responses by demographic subgroups.
a. Demographics of perceived winter warming
Figure 3a averages North Country winter temperature anomalies by decade (winter of 1897/98–1906/07 to 2007/08–2016/17), to match the wording of our survey question. By a substantial margin, winters over the past 20 years had been the warmest in this 120-yr record. Figure 3b charts the percentage of survey respondents aware of this warming, across demographic subgroups. Warming is recognized by majorities of every age group; by men and women; at each level of education; and by Democrats, Independents, and non–Tea Party Republicans. Tea Party supporters form the only major subgroup of North Country respondents in which most people do not recognize winter warming. A 31-point partisan gradient separates Democrats (70% recognize winter warming) from Tea Party supporters (just 39% do so). Partisan identity, alone among characteristics charted in Fig. 3b, correlates significantly with recognition of winter warming. The party/winter association fits with hypothesis H1.

(a) North Country winter temperature anomalies by decade, and (b) percentage of survey respondents who acknowledge winter warming broken down by age, sex, education, or political party, shown with probabilities from adjusted Wald tests.
Citation: Weather, Climate, and Society 10, 4; 10.1175/WCAS-D-18-0020.1

(a) North Country winter temperature anomalies by decade, and (b) percentage of survey respondents who acknowledge winter warming broken down by age, sex, education, or political party, shown with probabilities from adjusted Wald tests.
Citation: Weather, Climate, and Society 10, 4; 10.1175/WCAS-D-18-0020.1
(a) North Country winter temperature anomalies by decade, and (b) percentage of survey respondents who acknowledge winter warming broken down by age, sex, education, or political party, shown with probabilities from adjusted Wald tests.
Citation: Weather, Climate, and Society 10, 4; 10.1175/WCAS-D-18-0020.1
Our winter question does not mention climate change, but the subsequently-asked climate question does. Overall, 65% of North Country respondents agree with the scientific consensus that humans are changing Earth’s climate; this proportion falls close to results from recent nationwide surveys that asked the same question (Hamilton 2017). The partisan gradient in North Country climate responses (not shown) is even steeper than that for winter, ranging from 85% agreement with the scientific consensus among Democrats to 37% among Tea Party supporters. Partisan gradients in the same direction for both winter and climate are consistent with hypothesis H2.
b. Regression analysis
Table 2 presents results from probability-weighted logit regression models formally testing political party alongside other respondent characteristics as possible predictors of winter and climate responses. Age and years lived in the North Country exhibit no detectable effects on awareness of winter warming, in contradiction to hypothesis H4. Political identity, on the other hand, shows strong effects. Even controlling for other background factors, Republicans and especially Tea Party supporters are less inclined to acknowledge that recent winters have been warmer, or that human-caused climate change is real. These results offer further support for H1 and H2.
Respondent characteristics as predictors of belief that northern New England winters have been warmer over the past 20 years and that climate change is happening now, caused mainly by human activities. Given are the coefficients, standard errors (SE), and odds ratios from weighted logit regressions (n = 1443). A single asterisk indicates p < 0.05, two asterisks indicate p < 0.01, and three asterisks indicate p < 0.001, on the basis of F statistics from adjusted Wald tests.


Respondent education and having friends mostly of the same party affect winter warming responses through their interactions with party, described in the next section. The main effects of education are not significant for either winter or climate. Given the way our variables are centered, these main effects for education describe the impact of education among political Independents—that is, when party equals zero. Similarly the main effects of friends describe the impact of having mostly same-party friends, among Independents. Having a politically homogeneous peer group does not significantly affect the winter responses of Independents, although it does make them more likely to agree with the scientific consensus on climate change.
c. Interaction effects
Education × party (and similar) interactions have been widely confirmed with regard to climate change views in general. Typically, acceptance of anthropogenic climate change rises with education among Democrats and Independents (or liberals and moderates), but does not rise and may even decline with education among conservatives (Bolin and Hamilton 2018; Drummond and Fischoff 2017; Hamilton 2008, 2011; Hamilton et al. 2015; Kahan et al. 2012; McCright 2011; McCright and Dunlap 2011; Shao et al. 2014). The education × party interaction effect on climate in Table 2 conforms to this pattern. The weaker but also significant education × party effect on winter extends this phenomenon to perceptions about local weather. Awareness of winter warming in the North Country rises with education among Democrats, is unrelated to education among Independents, and declines with education among Republicans and Tea Party supporters. In this key detail, political effects on winter perceptions resemble political effects on climate-change opinions, which is consistent with hypothesis H2.
One novel result in Table 2 is that having friends mostly in the same party steepens the effect of political identity on both winter perceptions and climate views, even after controlling for the well-known education × party effect. This supports hypothesis H3, derived from previous reports of American trends toward same-party friends exacerbating polarization. Figure 4 visualizes the friends × party interactions through margins plots that adjust for other predictors in Table 2. Recognition of winter warming is higher among Democrats and Independents with most friends of the same persuasion than it is among their counterparts with mixed friends. Among Republicans and especially Tea Party supporters, the opposite is true: having friends mostly of the same party decreases recognition of factual warming, compared with their counterparts who have mixed friends (Fig. 4a). Similar observations can be made regarding the impacts of peer-group homogeneity on climate views, although in that case it makes somewhat less difference; acceptance or rejection of anthropogenic climate change is strongly linked to partisan identity regardless of friends’ leanings (Fig. 4b).

Interaction between respondent’s political party and most friends belonging to same party, affecting the probability they (a) recognize winter warming or (b) think climate change is happening now, caused mainly by human activities. Calculated from logit models in Table 2.
Citation: Weather, Climate, and Society 10, 4; 10.1175/WCAS-D-18-0020.1

Interaction between respondent’s political party and most friends belonging to same party, affecting the probability they (a) recognize winter warming or (b) think climate change is happening now, caused mainly by human activities. Calculated from logit models in Table 2.
Citation: Weather, Climate, and Society 10, 4; 10.1175/WCAS-D-18-0020.1
Interaction between respondent’s political party and most friends belonging to same party, affecting the probability they (a) recognize winter warming or (b) think climate change is happening now, caused mainly by human activities. Calculated from logit models in Table 2.
Citation: Weather, Climate, and Society 10, 4; 10.1175/WCAS-D-18-0020.1
We also tested an alternative specification of the winter model in Table 2 by including climate as an additional predictor (not shown). The strong relationship between party identification and climate-change beliefs makes their separate effects hard to interpret, and coefficient estimates less precise. Education × party and friends × party interactions remain significant, however, and have the same general meaning under either specification.
4. Discussion
Warming North Country winters, which have broad impacts across the economy, recreation, and everyday life, were noticed by a majority of residents young and old, male and female, with little or much education—but not by the most conservative. As visualized in Figs. 3 and 4, the nonperception of warming among this subgroup is strikingly distinct. Responses to our winter warming question, which does not mention climate change, are predicted not just by political party, but by the interaction of party with education. Similar interactions have been widely observed on questions explicitly about anthropogenic climate change, including one asked later on this same survey. Our results fit with previous studies that found conservatives reporting past weather consistent with their beliefs about climate change, while extending that finding to rural northern winters. Also noteworthy is the lack of support for the hypothesis that older or long-term residents in this region would be more aware of winter warming.
Most respondents to our survey reported accurately that recent winters have warmed, and most also agreed with scientists that human activities are changing Earth’s climate. The overlap between these groups somewhat limits the scope for experienced weather or climate to shape climate beliefs; many people who already think climate is changing notice change-consistent phenomena, whereas many of those who reject climate change do not. That dynamic helps to explain why climate-linked events such as hurricanes seem to have little immediate impact on public beliefs (Hamilton 2017). Physical events could nevertheless motivate action by those who recognize climate change already, and shift perceptions among people with less firm views, even while leaving many who firmly reject climate change unmoved.
One new result from the North Country survey is that area residents who primarily associate with people in their own political party tend to have more identity-consistent perceptions of weather. The causal interpretation of this result is ambiguous. Do people hold more partisan-shaded views about local weather because their friends reinforce that perspective? Or do people with overall more partisan outlooks, affecting even their perceptions about weather, also tend to select like-minded friends? Studies by the Pew Research Center (2016, 2017) report a parallel and similarly ambiguous finding that people whose friends are from the same political party tend to have stronger negative feelings about the other party and its people. Causality may operate in both directions, with choice of friends reflecting and amplifying partisan tendencies—another instance of the feedbacks exacerbating U.S. political divisions, but here occurring in rural communities on the mundane topic of local weather. The friends × party interaction invites wider replication to identify other situations where it operates, or does not.
Impacts already observed in the North Country make area residents stakeholders in global change, and hint at future impacts to come. The region’s tourism industry, especially winter recreation, offers hope for development of postresource economies but depends upon weather and climate (Dawson and Scott 2013). Cold season recreation visits to the northeastern United States are projected to decline through the end of the century as shorter, low, or marginal snow seasons become more frequent under both lower- and higher-emission scenarios (Wobus et al. 2017). In the short term, North Country visitations might benefit or at least remain level as areas farther south in New England and the Mid-Atlantic become less suitable for snow sports. Such activities at higher elevations of northern interior New England could remain operational for longer, particularly in cases such as downhill skiing (but not snowmobiling) that can be supported by snowmaking (Dawson and Scott 2013; Scott et al. 2008; Wobus et al. 2017). Other observed or projected consequences of winter warming include greater warm-season prevalence of insects harmful to forests, wildlife, and humans (DeSantis et al. 2013; Ogden et al. 2014; Paradis et al. 2008), which may affect prospects for summer tourism and amenity development as well. North Country residents have many practical reasons to be concerned about climate change, and to support broad efforts toward mitigation such as renewable energy development (Hamilton et al. 2018). Notwithstanding some politically based resistance, our survey found high overall awareness that humans are changing Earth’s climate, with local conditions shifting as well.
Acknowledgments
The North Country survey was supported by a grant from the Neil and Louise Tillotson Fund of the New Hampshire Charitable Foundation. The Survey Center at the University of New Hampshire performed sampling and interviews. Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this paper are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of supporting organizations.
REFERENCES
AAPOR, 2016: Standard Definitions: Final Disposition of Case Codes and Outcome Rates for Surveys (2016 revision). T. W. Smith, Ed., American Association for Public Opinion Research, 80 pp.
Akerlof, K., E. W. Maibach, D. Fitzgerald, A. Cedeno, and A. Neuman, 2013: Do people “personally experience” global warming, and if so how, and does it matter? Global Environ. Change, 23, 81–91, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2012.07.006.
Baily, M. N., and B. P. Bosworth, 2014: US manufacturing: Understanding its past and its potential future. J. Econ. Perspect., 28, 3–26, https://doi.org/10.1257/jep.28.1.3.
Bever, F., 2017: New England’s ski industry prepares for a changing climate. Maine Public Radio, 27 March 2017, http://mainepublic.org/post/new-englands-ski-industry-prepares-changing-climate.
Bolin, J. L., and L. C. Hamilton, 2018: The news you choose: News media preferences amplify views on climate change. Environ. Polit., 27, 455–476, https://doi.org/10.1080/09644016.2018.1423909.
Borick, C. P., and B. G. Rabe, 2010: A reason to believe: Examining the factors that determine individual views on global warming. Soc. Sci. Quart., 91, 777–800, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6237.2010.00719.x.
Borick, C. P., and B. G. Rabe, 2017: Personal experience, extreme weather events, and perceptions of climate change. Oxford Research Encyclopedias: Climate Science, https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228620.013.311.
Broomell, S. B., J.-F. Winkles, and P. B. Kane, 2017: The perception of daily temperatures as evidence of global warming. Wea. Climate Soc., 9, 563–574, https://doi.org/10.1175/WCAS-D-17-0003.1.
Brulle, R. J., J. Carmichael, and J. C. Jenkins, 2012: Shifting public opinion on climate change: An empirical assessment of factors influencing concern over climate change in the U.S., 2002–2010. Climatic Change, 114, 169–188, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-012-0403-y.
Burakowski, E., and M. Magnusson, 2012: Climate impacts on the winter tourism economy in the United States. Natural Resources Defense Council, 33 pp., https://www.nrdc.org/resources/climate-impacts-winter-tourism-economy-united-states.
Capstick, S. B., and N. F. Pidgeon, 2014: Public perception of cold weather events as evidence for and against climate change. Climatic Change, 122, 695–708, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-013-1003-1.
Carlton, J. S., A. S. Mase, C. L. Knutson, M. C. Lemos, T. Haigh, D. P. Todey, and L. S. Prokopy, 2016: The effects of extreme drought on climate change beliefs, risk perceptions, and adaptation attitudes. Climatic Change, 135, 211–226, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-015-1561-5.
Carmichael, J. T., R. J. Brulle, and J. K. Huxster, 2017: The great divide: Understanding the role of media and other drivers of the partisan divide in public concern over climate change in the USA, 2001–2014. Climatic Change, 141, 599–612, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-017-1908-1.
Colocousis, C. R., and J. Young, 2011: Continuity and change in Coös County: Results from the 2010 North Country CERA survey. Carsey Institute, University of New Hampshire, 6 pp., http://scholars.unh.edu/carsey/143/.
Cook, J., and Coauthors, 2016: Consensus on consensus: A synthesis of consensus estimates on human-caused global warming. Environ. Res. Lett., 11, 048002, https://doi.org/10.1088/1748-9326/11/4/048002.
Cutler, M. J., 2015: Seeing and believing: The emergent nature of extreme weather perceptions. Environ. Sociol., 1, 293–303, https://doi.org/10.1080/23251042.2015.1085117.
Cutler, M. J., 2016: Class, ideology, and severe weather: How the interaction of social and physical factors shape climate change threat perceptions among coastal US residents. Environ. Sociol., 2, 275–285, https://doi.org/10.1080/23251042.2016.1210842.
Dawson, J., and D. Scott, 2013: Managing for climate change in the alpine ski sector. Tour. Manage., 35, 244–254, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tourman.2012.07.009.
Dawson, J., D. Scott, and M. Havitz, 2013: Skier demand and behavioural adaptation to climate change in the US Northeast. Leisure/Loisir, 37, 127–143, https://doi.org/10.1080/14927713.2013.805037.
DeSantis, R. D., W. K. Moser, D. D. Gormanson, M. G. Bartlett, and B. Vermunt, 2013: Effects of climate on emerald ash borer mortality and the potential for ash survival in North America. Agric. For. Meteor., 178–179, 120–128, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.agrformet.2013.04.015.
Drummond, C., and B. Fischhoff, 2017: Individuals with greater science literacy and education have more polarized beliefs on controversial science topics. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 114, 9587–9592, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1704882114.
Dudley, R. W., G. A. Hodgkins, M. R. McHale, M. J. Kolian, and B. Renard, 2017: Trends in snowmelt-related streamflow timing in the conterminous United States. J. Hydrol., 547, 208–221, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhydrol.2017.01.051.
Egan, P. J., and M. Mullin, 2012: Turning personal experience into political attitudes: The effect of local weather on Americans’ perceptions about global warming. J. Polit., 74, 796–809, https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022381612000448.
Fogg, L. M., L. Hamilton, and C. Grimm, 2017: Views from the North Country: Surveying residents of northern New England. Carsey School of Public Policy, University of New Hampshire, 98 pp., https://carsey.unh.edu/sites/carsey.unh.edu/files/media/pdf/2017_north_country_full_results_report.pdf.
Glasmeier, A., and P. Salant, 2006: Low-skill workers in rural America face permanent job loss. Carsey Institute, University of New Hampshire, 8 pp., https://scholars.unh.edu/carsey/6/.
Hamilton, L. C., 2008: Who cares about polar regions? Results from a survey of U.S. public opinion. Arct. Antarct. Alp. Res., 40, 671–678, https://doi.org/10.1657/1523-0430(07-105)[HAMILTON]2.0.CO;2.
Hamilton, L. C., 2011: Education, politics and opinions about climate change: Evidence for interaction effects. Climatic Change, 104, 231–242, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-010-9957-8.
Hamilton, L. C., 2016: Public awareness of the scientific consensus on climate. SAGE Open, 6 (4), https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244016676296.
Hamilton, L. C., 2017: Data snapshot: Public acceptance of human-caused climate change is gradually rising. Carsey School of Public Policy, University of New Hampshire, 1 p., http://scholars.unh.edu/carsey/322/.
Hamilton, L. C., and B. D. Keim, 2009: Regional variation in perceptions about climate change. Int. J. Climatol., 29, 2348–2352, https://doi.org/10.1002/joc.1930.
Hamilton, L. C., and M. D. Stampone, 2013: Blowin’ in the wind: Short-term weather and belief in anthropogenic climate change. Wea. Climate Soc., 5, 112–119, https://doi.org/10.1175/WCAS-D-12-00048.1.
Hamilton, L. C., and M. Lemcke-Stampone, 2014: Arctic warming and your weather: Public belief in the connection. Int. J. Climatol., 34, 1723–1728, https://doi.org/10.1002/joc.3796.
Hamilton, L. C., and K. Saito, 2015: A four-party view of US environmental concern. Environ. Polit., 24, 212–227, https://doi.org/10.1080/09644016.2014.976485.
Hamilton, L. C., and M. Lemcke-Stampone, 2016: Was December warm? Family, politics, and recollections of weather. Carsey School of Public Policy, University of New Hampshire, 7 pp., http://scholars.unh.edu/carsey/276/.
Hamilton, L. C., D. E. Rohall, B. C. Brown, G. Hayward, and B. D. Keim, 2003: Warming winters and New Hampshire’s lost ski areas: An integrated case study. Int. J. Sociol. Soc. Policy, 23, 52–73, https://doi.org/10.1108/01443330310790309.
Hamilton, L. C., B. C. Brown, and B. D. Keim, 2007: Ski areas, weather and climate: Time series models for New England case studies. Int. J. Climatol., 27, 2113–2124, https://doi.org/10.1002/joc.1502.
Hamilton, L. C., L. R. Hamilton, C. M. Duncan, and C. R. Colocousis, 2008: Place matters: Challenges and opportunities in four rural Americas. Carsey Institute, University of New Hampshire, 32 pp., http://scholars.unh.edu/carsey/41/.
Hamilton, L. C., J. Hartter, M. Lemcke-Stampone, D. W. Moore, and T. G. Safford, 2015: Tracking public beliefs about anthropogenic climate change. PLOS ONE, 10, e0138208, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0138208.
Hamilton, L. C., J. Hartter, B. D. Keim, A. E. Boag, M. W. Palace, F. R. Stevens, and M. J. Ducey, 2016a: Wildfire, climate, and perceptions in northeast Oregon. Reg. Environ. Change, 16, 1819–1832, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10113-015-0914-y.
Hamilton, L. C., C. P. Wake, J. Hartter, T. G. Safford, and A. Puchlopek, 2016b: Flood realities, perceptions, and the depth of divisions on climate. Sociology, 50, 913–933, https://doi.org/10.1177/0038038516648547.
Hamilton, L. C., L. M. Fogg, and C. Grimm, 2017: Challenge and hope in the North Country. Carsey School of Public Policy, University of New Hampshire, 10 pp., http://scholars.unh.edu/carsey/326/.
Hamilton, L. C., E. Bell, J. Hartter, and J. D. Salerno, 2018: A change in the wind? U.S. public views on renewable energy and climate compared. Energy Sustain. Soc., 8 (11), https://doi.org/10.1186/s13705-018-0152-5.
Hodgkins, G. A., 2013: The importance of record length in estimating the magnitude of climatic changes: An example using 175 years of lake ice-out dates in New England. Climatic Change, 119, 705–718, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-013-0766-8.
Howe, P. D., and A. Leiserowitz, 2013: Who remembers a hot summer or a cold winter? The asymmetric effect of beliefs about global warming on perceptions of local climate conditions in the U.S. Global Environ. Change, 23, 1488–1500, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2013.09.014.
IPCC, 2013: Climate Change: The Physical Science Basis. T. F. Stocker et al., Eds., Cambridge University Press, 1535 pp.
Iyengar, S., and S. J. Westwood, 2015: Fear and loathing across party lines: New evidence on group polarization. Amer. J. Pol. Sci., 59, 690–707, https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12152.
Joireman, J., H. B. Truelove, and B. Duell, 2010: Effect of outdoor temperature, heat primes and anchoring on belief in global warming. J. Environ. Psychol., 30, 358–367, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2010.03.004.
Kahan, D. M., E. Peters, M. Wittlin, P. Slovic, L. L. Ouellette, D. Braman, and G. Mandel, 2012: The polarizing impact of science literacy and numeracy on perceived climate change risks. Nat. Climate Change, 2, 732–735, https://doi.org/10.1038/nclimate1547.
Kaufmann, R. K., M. L. Mann, S. Gopal, J. A. Liederman, P. D. Howe, F. Pretis, X. Tang, and M. Gilmore, 2017: Spatial heterogeneity of climate change as an experiential basis for skepticism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 114, 67–71, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1607032113.
Knowles, N., 2015: Trends in snow cover and related quantities at weather stations in the conterminous United States. J. Climate, 28, 7518–7528, https://doi.org/10.1175/JCLI-D-15-0051.1.
Krosnick, J. A., A. L. Holbrook, L. Lowe, and P. S. Visser, 2006: The origins and consequences of democratic citizens’ policy agendas: A study of popular concern about global warming. Climatic Change, 77, 7–43, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-006-9068-8.
Kunkel, K. E., M. Palecki, L. Ensor, K. G. Hubbard, D. Robinson, K. Redmond, and D. Easterling, 2009: Trends in twentieth-century U.S. snowfall using a quality-controlled dataset. J. Atmos. Oceanic Technol., 26, 33–44, https://doi.org/10.1175/2008JTECHA1138.1.
Kunkel, K. E., L. E. Stevens, S. E. Stevens, and L. Sun, 2013: Regional climate trends and scenarios for the U.S. National Climate Assessment. Part 1: Climate of the Northeast U.S. NOAA Tech. Rep. NESDIS 142-1, 79 pp., http://scenarios.globalchange.gov/sites/default/files/NOAA_NESDIS_Tech_Report_142-1-Climate_of_the_Northeast_U.S_1.pdf.
Kunkel, K. E., D. A. Robinson, S. Champion, X. Yin, T. Estilow, and R. M. Frankson, 2016: Trends and extremes in Northern Hemisphere snow characteristics. Curr. Climate Change Rep., 2, 65–73, https://doi.org/10.1007/s40641-016-0036-8.
Lee, T. M., E. M. Markowitz, P. D. Howe, C.-Y. Ko, and A. A. Leiserowitz, 2015: Predictors of public climate change awareness and risk perception around the world. Nat. Climate Change, 5, 1014–1020, https://doi.org/10.1038/nclimate2728.
Leiserowitz, A., E. Maibach, C. Roser-Renouf, and J. D. Hmielowski, 2011: Politics & global warming: Democrats, Republicans, Independents, and the Tea Party. Yale Project on Climate Change Communication, 34 pp., http://environment.yale.edu/climate/files/PoliticsGlobalWarming2011.pdf.
Li, Y., E. J. Johnson, and L. Zaval, 2011: Local warming: Daily temperature change influences belief in global warming. Psychol. Sci., 22, 454–459, https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611400913.
Maloney, E. D., and Coauthors, 2014: North American climate in CMIP5 experiments: Part III: Assessment of twenty-first-century projections. J. Climate, 27, 2230–2270, https://doi.org/10.1175/JCLI-D-13-00273.1.
Marlon, J. R., S. van der Linden, P. D. Howe, A. Leiserowitz, S. H. L. Woo, and K. Broad, 2018: Detecting local environmental change: The role of experience in shaping risk judgments about global warming. J. Risk Res., https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2018.1430051, in press.
Marquart-Pyatt, S. T., A. M. McCright, T. Dietz, and R. E. Dunlap, 2014: Politics eclipses climate extremes for climate change perceptions. Global Environ. Change, 29, 246–257, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2014.10.004.
McCright, A. M., 2011: Political orientation moderates Americans’ beliefs and concern about climate change. Climatic Change, 104, 243–253, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-010-9946-y.
McCright, A. M., and R. E. Dunlap, 2011: The politicization of climate change: Political polarization in the American public’s views of global warming, 2001–2010. Sociol. Quart., 52, 155–194, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1533-8525.2011.01198.x.
McCright, A. M., R. E. Dunlap, and C. Xiao, 2014: The impacts of temperature anomalies and political orientation on perceived winter warming. Nat. Climate Change, 4, 1077–1081, https://doi.org/10.1038/nclimate2443.
Myers, T. A., E. W. Mailbach, C. Roser-Renouf, K. Akerlof, and A. A. Leiserowitz, 2013: The relationship between personal experience and belief in the reality of global warming. Nat. Climate Change, 3, 343–347, https://doi.org/10.1038/nclimate1754.
NOAA, 2018: U.S. Climate Divisions dataset nClimDiv. Accessed 8 February 2018, https://www.ncdc.noaa.gov/monitoring-references/maps/us-climate-divisions.php.
Ogden, N. H., M. Radojevic, X. Wu, V. R. Duvvuri, P. A. Leighton, and J. Wu, 2014: Estimated effects of projected climate change on the basic reproductive number of the Lyme disease vector Ixodes scapularis. Environ. Health Perspect., 122, 631–638, https://doi.org/10.1289/ehp.1307799.
Paradis, A., J. Elkinton, K. Hayhoe, and J. Buonaccorsi, 2008: Role of winter temperature and climate change on the survival and future range expansion of the hemlock woolly adelgid (Adelges tsugae) in eastern North America. Mitig. Adapt. Strategies Global Change, 13, 541–554, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11027-007-9127-0.
Perry, E., R. Manning, X. Xiao, W. Valliere, and N. Reigner, 2018: Social climate change: The advancing extirpation of snowmobilers in Vermont. J. Park Recreation Admin., 36 (2), https://doi.org/10.18666/JPRA-2018-V36-I2-8307.
Pew Research Center, 2016: Partisanship and political animosity in 2016. Pew Research Center, accessed 28 December 2017, http://www.people-press.org/2016/06/22/partisanship-and-political-animosity-in-2016/.
Pew Research Center, 2017: The partisan divide on political values grows even wider. Pew Research Center, accessed 21 February 2018, http://www.people-press.org/2017/10/05/the-partisan-divide-on-political-values-grows-even-wider/.
Ripberger, J. T., H. C. Jenkins-Smith, C. L. Silva, D. E. Carlson, K. Gupta, N. Carlson, and R. E. Dunlap, 2017: Bayesian versus politically motivated reasoning in human perception of climate anomalies. Environ. Res. Lett., 12, 114004, https://doi.org/10.1088/1748-9326/aa8cfc.
Risen, J. L., and C. R. Critcher, 2011: Visceral fit: While in a visceral state, associated states of the world seem more likely. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol., 100, 777–793, https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022460.
Scott, D., J. Dawson, and B. Jones, 2008: Climate change vulnerability of the US Northeast winter recreation–tourism sector. Mitig. Adapt. Strategies Global Change, 13, 577–596, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11027-007-9136-z.
Shao, W., 2016: Are actual weather and perceived weather the same? Understanding perceptions of local weather and their effects on risk perceptions of global warming. J. Risk Res., 19, 722–742, https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2014.1003956.
Shao, W., 2017: Weather, climate, politics, or God? Determinants of American public opinions toward global warming. Environ. Polit., 26, 71–96, https://doi.org/10.1080/09644016.2016.1223190.
Shao, W., B. D. Keim, J. C. Garand, and L. C. Hamilton, 2014: Weather, climate, and the economy: Explaining risk perceptions of global warming, 2001–10. Wea. Climate Soc., 6, 119–134, https://doi.org/10.1175/WCAS-D-13-00029.1.
Shao, W., J. C. Garand, B. D. Keim, and L. C. Hamilton, 2016: Science, scientists, and local weather: Understanding mass perceptions of global warming. Soc. Sci. Quart., 97, 1023–1057, https://doi.org/10.1111/ssqu.12317.
Southwick Associates, 2017. The outdoor recreation economy. Outdoor Industry Association, 20 pp., https://outdoorindustry.org/wp-content/uploads/2017/04/OIA_RecEconomy_FINAL_Single.pdf.
Thibeault, J. M., and A. Seth, 2014: Changing climate extremes in the Northeast United States: Observations and projections from CMIP5. Climatic Change, 127, 273–287, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-014-1257-2.
U.S. EPA, 2016: Climate Change Indicators in the United States, 2016. 4th ed. Environmental Protection Agency Rep. EPA 430-R-16-004, 96 pp., www.epa.gov/climate-indicators.
USGCRP, 2017: Climate Science Special Report: Fourth National Climate Assessment, Volume I. U.S. Global Change Research Program, 470 pp., https://science2017.globalchange.gov/.
Vose, R. S., and Coauthors, 2014: NOAA’s Gridded Climate Divisional Dataset (CLIMDIV). NOAA National Climatic Data Center, https://doi.org/10.7289/V5M32STR.
Wake, C. P., E. Burakowski, P. Wilkinson, K. Hayhoe, A. Stoner, C. Keeley, and J. LaBranche, 2014: Climate change in northern New Hampshire: Past, present, and future. Climate Solutions New England, Sustainability Institute at the University of New Hampshire, 77 pp., http://sustainableunh.unh.edu/sites/sustainableunh.unh.edu/files/images/northernnhclimateassessment2014.pdf.
Weber, E. U., 2010: What shapes perceptions of climate change? Wiley Interdiscip. Rev.: Climate Change, 1, 332–342, https://doi.org/10.1002/wcc.41.
Wobus, C., and Coauthors, 2017: Projected climate change impacts on skiing and snowmobiling: A case study of the United States. Global Environ. Change, 45, 1–14, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2017.04.006.
Zaval, L., E. A. Keenan, E. J. Johnson, and E. U. Weber, 2014: How warm days increase belief in global warming. Nat. Climate Change, 4, 143–147, https://doi.org/10.1038/nclimate2093.